Interventions to Facilitate Communication in Autism
上传者:梅强|上传时间:2015-05-11|密次下载
Interventions to Facilitate Communication in Autism
自闭症儿童社交能力训练策略
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 30, No. 5, 2000
Interventions to Facilitate Communication in AutismLynn Kern Koegel1
The purpose of this article is to discuss research opportunities arising from the current liter-
ature in the area of communication. Six general themes are discussed, including (a) increas-
ing spontaneity, initiations, and the variety of functions of language verbal and nonverbal chil-
dren with autism exhibit; (b) assessing and teaching precursors relating to positive outcome;
(c) the importance of family involvement in intervention programs; (d) best practices for im-
plementation of communicative interventions; (e) the interrelationship between language and
other behavioral symptoms of autism; and (f) the social and pragmatic use of language. Theseareas are discussed in terms of improving assessment and intervention practices to produce
greater long-term communicative outcomes for individuals with autism.
KEY WORDS:Autism; communicative intervention.
INTRODUCTION
Researchers and practitioners report notable gains
in reducing the behavioral symptoms of children with
autism; however the literature suggests that there remains
a major need for research in social/communicative as-
pects of development. Few studies yield sufficient data
to adequately understand linguistic and communicative
development in children with autism (Dawson & Oster-
ling, 1997; Tager-Flusberg, 1989; Wetherby, 1998), even
though these areas appear to be related more directly to
clinical outcome than other characteristics of the dis-
ability (Venter, Lord, & Schopler, 1992). This article
suggests research opportunities that arise from directions
and gaps in the current literature. Six general themes that
appear to be relevant across ability levels are discussed:
(a) spontaneity, initiations, and functions of language;
(b) precursors related to positive outcomes and mea-
surement; (c) family involvement in intervention deli-
very; (d) implementation (when, where, what, and how
much); (e) the interrelationship between language and
other areas; and (f) the social/pragmatic use of language.
Some of these research opportunities result from
the heterogeneity of communication skills and deficits
1University of California, Santa Barbara, California.across children diagnosed as having autism. Althoughformal definitions of diagnostic entities exist, the bound-aries are unclear, and in addition to behavioral overlapwith other disabilities, the degree of impairment variesconsiderably depending on the intensity of expressionof symptoms (Waterhouse, Wing, & Fein, 1989). Con-founding clinical applications of research findings isthe lack of reliable and standard diagnostic formu-lations of communicative behavior to ensure compa-rability of populations (Lord, 1999). Without such measures, describing outcomes in terms of specific de-velopmental gains is difficult. Furthermore, it is verytime consuming to implement a thorough evaluation ofsocial and communicative areas and to accurately as-sess the interrelationship among the triad of impair-ments of the disability (Volden & Lord, 1991), particu-larly because communication varies in different contexts,among various communicative partners, and over time.However, there is a large and growing corpus of rig-orous single-subject experimental studies and somegroup comparisons documenting individual differencesin outcomes that contribute to an understanding of cur-rent research opportunities and gaps.A review of programs by Prizant (1983) suggestedthat, even with intervention, approximately 50% of chil-dren diagnosed as having autism remained nonverbal.
383
0162/3257/00/1000-0383$18.00/0 ©2000 Plenum Publishing Corporation
自闭症儿童社交能力训练策略
384
However, with improved teaching approaches (based onthe early work of Hart & Risley, 1978, with disadvan-taged children), especially those that systematically in-corporate motivational techniques, as many as 85–90%of children diagnosed as having autism, who begin inter-vention before the age of 5, can learn to use verbal com-munication as a primary mode of communication(Koegel, 1995; McGee, Daly, & Jacobs, 1994). Theseprocedures, resulting in substantially greater percent-ages of children who are able to use verbal expressivewords and language as their primary mode of commu-nication, focus on following the child’s lead (Yoder,Kaiser, Alpert, & Fischer, 1993), capitalize on thechild’s motivation to respond (R.L. Koegel, O’Dell, &Koegel, 1987) and provide frequent opportunities forexpressive language in natural environments through-out the day (Warren, McQuarter, & Rogers-Warren,1984) using direct and natural reinforcers (Charlop,Schreibman, & Thibodeau, 1985). When procedures in-corporating these variables are combined into an inter-vention package, children with autism who are initiallynonverbal increase their levels of immediate and de-ferred verbal imitation, word production, and sponta-neous utterances more than when the approach is amore structured discrete trial format (R.L. Koegeletal.,1987).
Researchers have replicated these effects with ver-bal children with autism who demonstrate delayed lan-guage. For example, R.L. Koegel, Koegel, and Surratt(1992), using an alternating treatments design, com-pared a motivational condition which closely approxi-mated natural language interactions with an analog con-dition consisting of a structured discrete trial format.This research found greater improvements in verbal at-tempts, word approximations, word production, andword combinations, as well as lower levels of disrup-tive behavior during the motivational teaching condi-tions when compared to the analog conditions. There-fore, for young children severely affected by autismwho are nonverbal or significantly delayed in the ac-quisition of expressive language, preliminary studiessuggest that such motivational procedures are particu-larly effective in early intervention (McGee, Feldman,& Maurier, 1997).
Related to this, a number of studies support theuse of such motivational teaching procedures to im-prove generalization outside of the intervention setting.Specifically, although children with autism appear todemonstrate steady and consistent gains during clini-cal sessions using highly structured analog procedures,they frequently fail to demonstrate generalization out-
side of the clinical setting in areas such as phonologyKoegel(R.L. Koegel, Camarata, Koegel, Ben-Tal, & Smith,1998) and syntax (McGee, Krantz, & McClannahan,1985) if naturalistic and motivational teaching proce-dures are not incorporated. Therefore, early and improvedintervention using a behavioral model, with specificcomponents of the procedure altered from a structuredanalog procedure to improve motivational areas suchas responsivity, rate of responding, and affect, has beenshown to be effective in increasing the number of chil-dren who become verbal and who demonstrate com-municative gains (R.L. Koegel et al.,1987; McGee,Morrier, & Daly, 1999).When one reviews overall patterns of improve-ment in scientifically documented intervention studies,the following six areas emerge as especially promisingresearch opportunities.SPONTANEITY, INITIATIONS, ANDFUNCTIONS OF LANGUAGEIn the area of autism, in contrast to other disabil-ities, lack of spontaneous verbal and nonverbal initia-tions appears to be a consistent problem. Regardless ofcommunicative level, the number of communicativeinitiations and the expansion of specific functions oflanguage seems to be particularly problematic. Thus,whether a child with autism uses verbal means of com-munication or uses augmentative and/or alternative sys-tems of communication, intervention research focusedon improving these skills is especially important.For example, consider the small percentage of chil-dren who remain nonverbal and for whom a number ofaugmentative and alternative communicative (AAC) sys-tems are now available. AAC systems can include signlanguage (Carr & Kologinsky, 1983; Horner & Day,1991), photographs (Kozleski, 1991; Reichle et al.,1996), picture exchange systems (Bondy & Frost,1998), communication books (Hunt, Alwell, Goetz, &Sailor, 1990), computer systems, and so on. Althoughteaching technologies are more clearly defined, spon-taneity has been a problem. While these nonverbal chil-dren can show large degrees of success with initial ac-quisition, they exhibit difficulties with generalizationof AAC systems (Mirenda & Mathy-Laikko, 1989;Schlosser, Belfloire, Nigam, Blischak, & Hetzroni,1997; Storey & Provost, 1996; Stiebel, 1999; Udwin &Yule, 1990). They often require constant and consistentadult prompting and fail to demonstrate the spontaneoususe of a variety of functions needed for communicativecompetence. Scientifically sound proceduresrelated toteaching the spontaneous use of AAC outside of a clin-
自闭症儿童社交能力训练策略
Interventions to Facilitate Communication in Autismical setting are clearly lacking in the literature. Althoughthe likelihood of generalization from clinic to homesettings increases if AAC is incorporated into familyroutines (Steibel, 1999),assessing the specific types ofcommunicative intentions that are most useful, perhapsby incorporating a functional analysis, may increase thechild’s use of AAC while reducing the demands on aparent or adult. Also, apraxia and difficulty with finemotor skills appear to be common in nonverbal chil-dren with autism who demonstrate difficulty with ACCsystems (Seal & Bonvillian, 1997). Thus, research onthe interrelationships between motor performance andcommunication may help determine the most appro-priate AAC systems for nonverbal children who fail toacquire verbal language.
Similar issues regarding the lack of spontaneity,initiations, and functions of language exist for verbalchildren with autism. Wetherby and Prutting (1984) an-alyzed language samples of verbal children with autismand found them limited in both quantity and quality.Children with autism use language almost exclusivelyfor requesting objects, requesting actions, and protest-ing. A growing body of research is demonstrating ef-fective procedures for teaching children with autismto use absent or depressed language functions such asasking questions (Hung, 1977; L.K. Koegel, Cama-rata, Valdez-Menchaca, & Koegel, 1998; Taylor &Harris, 1995; Warren, Baxter, Anderson, Marshall, & Baer, 1981) and other verbal initiations (L. Koegel,Koegel, Shoshan, & McNerney, 1999). Spontaneousinitiations are necessary for children with autism tobe judged as socially competent during interactions.Research designed to develop intervention proceduresto teach spontaneity and initiations and research as-sessing and defining the types and numbers of initia-tions children need to exhibit to appear communica-tively competent will enhance our understanding ofbest intervention practices.
PRECURSORS RELATED TO POSITIVEOUTCOMES AND MEASUREMENT
The literature suggests that the single most im-portant prognostic indicator for young children withautism is language ability (Schreibman, 1988). Specificinformation regarding the types and amounts of lan-guage precursors that lead to positive outcomes, bothfor verbal and preverbal children with autism, shouldgreatly enhance our research base. For example, jointattention is consistently noted as important in prelin-
guistic development. For typical language learners the385onset of joint attention appears to signal a new level ofunderstanding, and to have an intrinsic motivationalbase, which is social in nature and is strongly corre-lated with the emergence of naming (Bates, Thal, &Marchman, 1991). Unfortunately, children with autismgenerally demonstrate a lack of, or a reduced numberof, important referential gestures, such as showing, gi-ving, and pointing, and using eye gaze to communicate(Stone, Ousley, Yoder, Hogan, & Hepburn, 1997). Thisdeficit can be noted early in life in children with dis-abilities and appears to be an important milestone war-ranting research attention.For verbal children with autism, a similar perva-sive lack of social initiations appears to be a consistentpattern and likely precursor to communicative impair-ment during social interactions. A common utteranceduring initial lexical development in typical languagedevelopment is “that?” (/daet/), often used in conjunc-tion with pointing to items. This interrogative verbal-ization along with nonverbal cues, such as pointing,can be a specific prompt for a parent to label the itemfor the child (Halliday, 1975). Over time, this type ofutterance becomes increasingly more sophisticated andvaried, resulting in steadily more sophisticated questions(who, where, when, why, etc.). Rather than exhibitingthese typical developmental linguistic milestones, chil-dren with autism emit fewer utterances and fail to uselanguage as a means of social initiation (Calloway,Myles, & Earles, 1999).In a preliminary study, L. Koegel et al.(1999) com-pared archival data of adolescents who were diagnosedas having autism with extremely favorable versus ex-tremely poor outcomes following intervention. Analy-ses included pragmatic ratings during social interactions,school and community functioning (including academicfunctioning, social circles, and extracurricular activities),and adaptive behavior composites. At baseline, when thechildren were preschoolers, all exhibited characteristicsassociated with the likelihood of a favorable postinter-vention outcome, such as high verbal intelligence quo-tients (IQ above 60) and the presence of functionalspeech before 5 years of age (DeMyer et al.,1973; Free-man, Ritvo, Needleman, & Yokota, 1985; Gillberg &Steffenburg, 1987; Lotter, 1974; Venter et al.,1992).However, after long-term intensive intervention pro-grams, which were similar across all participants, thedata showed very different outcomes. Preliminary dataanalyses suggested that the children who had the mostpositive outcomes also exhibited the greatest numbersof social initiations prior to intervention, whereas thechildren with the least favorable outcomes exhibited fewor no social initiations prior to intervention.
自闭症儿童社交能力训练策略
386
Such assessment data on joint attention and initi-ations have led researchers to begin to develop inter-vention procedures to teach these potentially importantprerequisites. For example, Yoder and Warren (1999)have begun to develop interventions to increase thegeneralized use of self-initiated proto-declaratives andproto-imperatives in prelinguistic children with devel-opmental disabilities (some of whom present withsymptoms of autism). Others (Hung, 1977; L. Koegel,Camarata, Valdez-Menchaca & Koegel, 1998; Taylor& Harris, 1995; Warren et al.,1981) have designed in-tervention programs to improve the use of self-initiatedquestions and other spontaneous verbalizations. Pre-liminary data suggest that teaching these linguisticskills may result in more widespread clinical gains andmore favorable long-term outcomes.
The literature strongly suggests that without ade-quate early social input, the neurological and behavioraldevelopment of children with autism may be further de-flected from a typical developmental path (Mundy &Crowson, 1997). There appear to be large individual dif-ferences in joint attention and social-verbal initiationsacross children diagnosed as having autism. Improvedprecision in measuring the nature of early social com-munication in children with autism, as well as compre-hensive outcome measures, appears to be a productiveavenue for future research.
It is important, however, that information regar-ding prognostic indicators not be used to deny servicesto very young children who do not exhibit positive in-dicators (Parisy, 1999). Instead, determination of thevariety of verbal and nonverbal behaviors that lead toimproved prognoses through more precise and compre-hensive longitudinal studies should help improve out-comes. Research identifying critical communicative be-haviors that need to be targeted is likely to result in moreefficient interventions for greater numbers of children.FAMILY INVOLVEMENT IN
INTERVENTION DELIVERY
Schopler and Reichler (1971) pointed out in theearly 1970s that parents of children with autism are crit-ical components of the habilitation process, withoutwhom gains are unlikely to be maintained (Lovaas,Koegel, Simmons, & Long, 1973). Unfortunately, manylanguage programs are implemented without involvingfamily members, even though the transactional natureof communication suggests that appropriate modifica-tions of a caregiver’s interactive style may help chil-
dren to develop a sense of efficacy and competence inKoegelcommunicative development (Prizant & Wetherby,1998).With respect to the development of parent educa-tion programs, several important themes are emergingin the literature. First, it is well documented that thedemands associated with having a child with a disabil-ity greatly increase parental stress (Bristol & Schopler,1983; Moes, Koegel, Schreibman, & Loos, 1992). In oursociety, there is a lack of preparation, assistance, andopportunities for socialization for parents of childrenwith disabilities. Many parents of children with autismexperience a lack of welcome in public places, and theirfamilies’ social circles may change or decrease signif-icantly because of the challenging behaviors and delays their child exhibits.Lord and O’Neill (1983) pointed out that pro-gramming for children with autism is particularly dif-ficult because many of their deficits, such as languagedevelopment and socialization, are learned by typicallydeveloping children with little or no direct interven-tion. Research has demonstrated that parent educationprograms that obligate the parents to designate certaintime periods to work one-on-one with their children in-crease parental stress. However, parent education pro-grams, when implemented properly, can help to reduceparent stress while greatly improving communication(Moes, 1995). Programs designed to fit into families’lifestyles and routines, so that teaching can occur onan ongoing basis throughout the day in natural settings,actually decrease family stress while resulting in greatergains in child communication (Koegel, Bimbela, &Schreibman, 1996; Schreibman, Kaneko & Koegel,1991).In light of the documented need for coordinatedefforts with family members, some areas appear highlylikely to benefit from further research. For example, in-dividualizing the intervention to adjust for contextualvariables that may result in less effective parent edu-cation programs, such as poverty, social isolation, de-pression, stress, and marital discord, may be of greatimportance (Baker, 1984; Dumas & Wahler, 1983;Forehand, Furey, & McMahon, 1984; Plienis, Robbins,& Dunlap, 1988; Robbins, Dunlap, & Plienis, 1991).In addition, with increases in cultural diversity in oursociety, parent education programs that develop cul-turally and linguistically sensitive intervention plansmight also benefit from further research. Research re-garding the concepts of “goodness of fit” and “contex-tual fit” (Bailey et al.,1990), when incorporated intoresearch on intervention plans, may improve parent edu-cation programs, benefit the general family system,and the child’s overall functioning.
自闭症儿童社交能力训练策略
Interventions to Facilitate Communication in AutismIMPLEMENTATION (WHEN, WHERE, WHAT,AND HOW MUCH)
An issue that has created heated controversy in re-cent years relates to the quantity of implementation ofservices. Just a few decades ago, most children withautism received little or no systematic intervention incommunity settings. Now, the importance of intensiveand early intervention in natural settings has becomean especially salient issue (Lovaas, 1987). In the areaof communication, Hart and Risley (1995) have docu-mented the importance of verbal stimulation on futuredevelopment for typically developing children. Unfor-tunately, children with autism often actively avoid so-cial interactions (Carr & Durand, 1985), thereby greatlyreducing the number of language learning and teach-ing opportunities (Peck, 1985). Even when languageteaching is provided, many programs do not work oncommunication in social and natural community set-tings. This has become increasingly important as chil-dren with autism are having more contact with typi-cally developing children as school and communityinclusion increases (Harrower, 1999; Kennedy, Shukla,& Fryxell, 1997; Strain, McGee, & Kohler, in press).
Best practices and procedures, so that childrenwith autism progress toward connected coherent socialdiscourse for communicative competence, are unknownat this time, and thus comprehensive investigation ofthis area is needed. Research regarding wheninterven-tion for linguistic and social development should takeplace, whereor in what settings intervention shouldoccur, how muchintervention is optimal, and whattypesof interventions need to be implemented for ac-celerated social and linguistic gains and communica-tive competence may be of considerable benefit to thefield. Certainly this issue relates to the individualiza-tion of intervention, as responsivity to different typesof interventions are likely to depend on particular childand family characteristics.
INTERRELATIONSHIP BETWEEN
LANGUAGE AND OTHER AREAS
The interrelationship between communicative de-ficits and other behavioral symptoms of autism hascome to the forefront of behavioral intervention. Epi-demiological studies suggest that as many as 75–80%of behavior problems may have a communicative func-tion (Derby et al.,1992; Iwata et al.,1994). Reportedchanges in behavior that occur when antecedent sti-
muli are addressed and functionally equivalent behaviors387are taught can be quite dramatic (Wacker, Berg, Asmus,Harding, & Cooper, 1998) and can improve generalfamily harmony and affect (L. Koegel, Steibel, & Koegel,1998). Furthermore, the reconceptualization of problembehavior as communicative rather than “maladaptive”or “noncompliant” has led researchers and practition-ers toward developing more functional and humaneintervention procedures. Similarly, researchers haverecently been attempting to define and quantify prag-matic areas of disability and the complex interactionbetween language, social, and cognitive development(Volden & Lord, 1991).Research relating to the understanding and as-sessment of interrelationships between communicationand behavior has been prolific in the last decade andhas created numerous new research opportunities forpromoting communication in autism. Assessing thefunctions of behavior problems is now a common prac-tice and an essential component of most behavioral sup-port plans, and a number of tools are readily availablethrough direct observation (Frea, Koegel, & Koegel,1993), interviews (O’Neill, Horner, Albin, Storey, &Newton, 1990), and review of records. However, thescience of teaching functionally equivalent behaviorsis less well defined. How often and at what times prac-titioners need to prompt functionally equivalent re-placement behaviors before behavior problems areeliminated is not yet clear. In addition, studies that im-prove our understanding of the social contexts that sup-port problem behaviors, particularly when there aremultiple maintaining variables, are needed. The rela-tionship between the acquisition and spontaneity offunctionally equivalent replacement behaviors is notwell understood. There appears to be an inverse rela-tionship between the acquisition of a functionallyequivalent replacement behavior and the decrease ofproblem behaviors. However, the exact interaction be-tween the appropriate and inappropriate behaviors maybe complex and dependent on how well established theproblem behavior is, the context in which the behaviorproblems occur, the child’s level of functioning, andother variables. The issue of transfer of interventiongains across community settings may be better under-stood with additional research. Although the essenceof teaching a functionally equivalent behavior is to pro-vide a socially appropriate mode of communication, ag-gression and self-injury often are effective and efficientin terms of communication. Therefore, even though in-appropriate, they may be well established in terms ofproducing desired outcomes. Areas such as efficiencyof communicative responses, reinforcement schedules,and delayed consequences are likely to have a direct
下载文档
热门试卷
- 2016年四川省内江市中考化学试卷
- 广西钦州市高新区2017届高三11月月考政治试卷
- 浙江省湖州市2016-2017学年高一上学期期中考试政治试卷
- 浙江省湖州市2016-2017学年高二上学期期中考试政治试卷
- 辽宁省铁岭市协作体2017届高三上学期第三次联考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市钦州港区2016-2017学年高二11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市钦州港区2017届高三11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市钦州港区2016-2017学年高一11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市高新区2016-2017学年高二11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市高新区2016-2017学年高一11月月考政治试卷
- 山东省滨州市三校2017届第一学期阶段测试初三英语试题
- 四川省成都七中2017届高三一诊模拟考试文科综合试卷
- 2017届普通高等学校招生全国统一考试模拟试题(附答案)
- 重庆市永川中学高2017级上期12月月考语文试题
- 江西宜春三中2017届高三第一学期第二次月考文科综合试题
- 内蒙古赤峰二中2017届高三上学期第三次月考英语试题
- 2017年六年级(上)数学期末考试卷
- 2017人教版小学英语三年级上期末笔试题
- 江苏省常州西藏民族中学2016-2017学年九年级思想品德第一学期第二次阶段测试试卷
- 重庆市九龙坡区七校2016-2017学年上期八年级素质测查(二)语文学科试题卷
- 江苏省无锡市钱桥中学2016年12月八年级语文阶段性测试卷
- 江苏省无锡市钱桥中学2016-2017学年七年级英语12月阶段检测试卷
- 山东省邹城市第八中学2016-2017学年八年级12月物理第4章试题(无答案)
- 【人教版】河北省2015-2016学年度九年级上期末语文试题卷(附答案)
- 四川省简阳市阳安中学2016年12月高二月考英语试卷
- 四川省成都龙泉中学高三上学期2016年12月月考试题文科综合能力测试
- 安徽省滁州中学2016—2017学年度第一学期12月月考高三英语试卷
- 山东省武城县第二中学2016.12高一年级上学期第二次月考历史试题(必修一第四、五单元)
- 福建省四地六校联考2016-2017学年上学期第三次月考高三化学试卷
- 甘肃省武威第二十三中学2016—2017学年度八年级第一学期12月月考生物试卷
网友关注
- 第八章wcdma系统无线接口
- WCDMA规划中容量与覆盖模型研究
- wcdma无线直放站的射频关键技术研究硕士论文
- 2011CCNP学习笔记一QOS
- wcdma无线网络规划课件
- WCDMA系统中HSPA%2b准入控制算法研究
- CDMA无线网优工程师技能考核规范指导书
- WCDMA无线网络优化的概述
- WCDMA切换专题[最新]
- wcdma系统中hsdpa技术的研究与应用
- WCDMA无线网络规划方法探究
- wcdma hspa+中64qam优化方案研究及性能验证
- CDMA网络无线性能的分析和优化
- 中电信战略明晰:量身定制全系列机型 CDMA手机市场繁荣
- WCDMA业务介绍14661
- 通信英文文献
- 3 CDMA系统规划
- 应用于wcdma系统的便携式无源互调传输测试系统的设计与实现
- 2GHz+WCDMA数字蜂窝移动通信网Iub接口技术要求第一阶段第3部分+信令传输
- 中兴合作单位WCDMA技能认证考试试题(中级)最新(可编辑)
- wcdma无线资源管理中软切换技术的研究
- GSM协议体系结构简介
- 电子与通信工程专业毕业论文 [精品论文] WCDMA定位技术与算法的研究
- wcdma无线网络优化方案研究
- WCDMA系统Iub接口流量控制算法研究与实现
- WCDMA无线资源管理算法的实现
- 中国联通CDMA直放站统一监控协议规范
- wcdma系统中turbo码的若干问题研究硕士论文
- 工商管理专业毕业论文 [精品论文] 中国联通wcdma第三代移动通信网络营销策略研究
- wcdma产品技术培训 25【精品】
网友关注视频
- 冀教版小学数学二年级下册第二单元《有余数除法的简单应用》
- 沪教版八年级下册数学练习册一次函数复习题B组(P11)
- 【部编】人教版语文七年级下册《泊秦淮》优质课教学视频+PPT课件+教案,广东省
- 8 随形想象_第一课时(二等奖)(沪教版二年级上册)_T3786594
- 【部编】人教版语文七年级下册《逢入京使》优质课教学视频+PPT课件+教案,安徽省
- 沪教版牛津小学英语(深圳用) 四年级下册 Unit 3
- 【部编】人教版语文七年级下册《老山界》优质课教学视频+PPT课件+教案,安徽省
- 外研版英语七年级下册module3 unit2第一课时
- 青岛版教材五年级下册第四单元(走进军营——方向与位置)用数对确定位置(一等奖)
- 二年级下册数学第一课
- 冀教版小学数学二年级下册第二单元《有余数除法的整理与复习》
- 精品·同步课程 历史 八年级 上册 第15集 近代科学技术与思想文化
- 8.练习八_第一课时(特等奖)(苏教版三年级上册)_T142692
- 沪教版牛津小学英语(深圳用) 四年级下册 Unit 7
- 【部编】人教版语文七年级下册《逢入京使》优质课教学视频+PPT课件+教案,安徽省
- 30.3 由不共线三点的坐标确定二次函数_第一课时(市一等奖)(冀教版九年级下册)_T144342
- 三年级英语单词记忆下册(沪教版)第一二单元复习
- 沪教版牛津小学英语(深圳用) 四年级下册 Unit 8
- 沪教版八年级下册数学练习册20.4(2)一次函数的应用2P8
- 沪教版八年级下册数学练习册21.3(3)分式方程P17
- 19 爱护鸟类_第一课时(二等奖)(桂美版二年级下册)_T3763925
- 冀教版小学数学二年级下册第二周第2课时《我们的测量》宝丰街小学庞志荣.mp4
- 外研版英语三起6年级下册(14版)Module3 Unit2
- 【部编】人教版语文七年级下册《老山界》优质课教学视频+PPT课件+教案,安徽省
- 第4章 幂函数、指数函数和对数函数(下)_六 指数方程和对数方程_4.7 简单的指数方程_第一课时(沪教版高一下册)_T1566237
- 第19课 我喜欢的鸟_第一课时(二等奖)(人美杨永善版二年级下册)_T644386
- 冀教版小学英语四年级下册Lesson2授课视频
- 《小学数学二年级下册》第二单元测试题讲解
- 沪教版牛津小学英语(深圳用) 四年级下册 Unit 4
- 北师大版八年级物理下册 第六章 常见的光学仪器(二)探究凸透镜成像的规律
精品推荐
- 2016-2017学年高一语文人教版必修一+模块学业水平检测试题(含答案)
- 广西钦州市高新区2017届高三11月月考政治试卷
- 浙江省湖州市2016-2017学年高一上学期期中考试政治试卷
- 浙江省湖州市2016-2017学年高二上学期期中考试政治试卷
- 辽宁省铁岭市协作体2017届高三上学期第三次联考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市钦州港区2016-2017学年高二11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市钦州港区2017届高三11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市钦州港区2016-2017学年高一11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市高新区2016-2017学年高二11月月考政治试卷
- 广西钦州市高新区2016-2017学年高一11月月考政治试卷
分类导航
- 互联网
- 电脑基础知识
- 计算机软件及应用
- 计算机硬件及网络
- 计算机应用/办公自动化
- .NET
- 数据结构与算法
- Java
- SEO
- C/C++资料
- linux/Unix相关
- 手机开发
- UML理论/建模
- 并行计算/云计算
- 嵌入式开发
- windows相关
- 软件工程
- 管理信息系统
- 开发文档
- 图形图像
- 网络与通信
- 网络信息安全
- 电子支付
- Labview
- matlab
- 网络资源
- Python
- Delphi/Perl
- 评测
- Flash/Flex
- CSS/Script
- 计算机原理
- PHP资料
- 数据挖掘与模式识别
- Web服务
- 数据库
- Visual Basic
- 电子商务
- 服务器
- 搜索引擎优化
- 存储
- 架构
- 行业软件
- 人工智能
- 计算机辅助设计
- 多媒体
- 软件测试
- 计算机硬件与维护
- 网站策划/UE
- 网页设计/UI
- 网吧管理